Hello

Your subscription is almost coming to an end. Don’t miss out on the great content on Nation.Africa

Ready to continue your informative journey with us?

Hello

Your premium access has ended, but the best of Nation.Africa is still within reach. Renew now to unlock exclusive stories and in-depth features.

Reclaim your full access. Click below to renew.

By age 6, girls less likely to believe they are 'brilliant'- study

A girl learns how to use a computer key board. Gender stereotypes emerge early, and by age six, girls are less likely to believe females are "brilliant" and more likely to believe boys are, according to a study released Thursday. AGENCIES PHOTO

What you need to know:

  • The findings may have important implications for the career paths that women choose, perhaps steering them away from fields typically associated with brilliance, like physics and philosophy, said lead author Lin Bian, a researcher at the University of Illinois, Urbana-Champlain.
  • In another part of the study, children had to guess which of four children, two boys and two girls, got the best grades in school.

Gender stereotypes emerge early, and by age six, girls are less likely to believe females are "brilliant" and more likely to believe boys are, according to a study released Thursday.
The report in the journal Science was based on 400 youths aged between five and seven, who were given a series of tasks.
In one, the kids were told a short story about a person who was "really, really smart," but were given no hints about whether the person was male or female.
At age five, both boys and girls were equally likely to choose their own gender as "really, really smart."
But by age six and seven, "girls were significantly less likely than boys to associate brilliance with their own gender," said the study.
In another part of the study, children had to guess which of four children, two boys and two girls, got the best grades in school.

In this case, both younger and older girls were just as likely to select girls as having top grades, suggesting that the "girls' perceptions of school achievement were separate from their perceptions of brilliance," said the report.
Finally, when the kids were asked about their interest in two new games, one for "children who are really, really smart" and the other for "children who try really, really hard," researchers found that girls aged six and seven were less interested than boys in the game for smart children.
At age five, girls were just as likely to pick the whiz kids' game as the game for kids who try hard.

The findings may have important implications for the career paths that women choose, perhaps steering them away from fields typically associated with brilliance, like physics and philosophy, said lead author Lin Bian, a researcher at the University of Illinois, Urbana-Champlain.
"It is likely to affect women's career aspirations," she said.
"We believe it is important to explore whether and how early young kids start learning these stereotypes associating brilliance with males."